Next Article in Journal
Potential Use of Kasedbok (Neptunia javanica Miq.) on Feed Intake, Digestibility, Rumen Fermentation, and Microbial Populations in Thai Native Beef Cattle
Next Article in Special Issue
The Effect of Enrichment on Leopard Geckos (Eublepharis macularius) Housed in Two Different Maintenance Systems (Rack System vs. Terrarium)
Previous Article in Journal
Characteristics of Microbiota in Different Segments of the Digestive Tract of Lycodon rufozonatus
Previous Article in Special Issue
Mobile Zoos and Other Itinerant Animal Handling Events: Current Status and Recommendations for Future Policies
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Defining Short-Term Accommodation for Animals

1
Emergent Disease Foundation, 71–75 Shelton Street, Covent Garden, London WC2H 9JQ, UK
2
Veterinary Expert, P.O. Box 575, Swansea SA8 9AW, UK
3
School of Applied Sciences, London South Bank University, 103 Borough Rd., London SE1 0AA, UK
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Animals 2023, 13(4), 732; https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13040732
Submission received: 7 November 2022 / Revised: 6 February 2023 / Accepted: 14 February 2023 / Published: 17 February 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Captive Animals: Perspectives, Practices, Challenges and Ethics)

Abstract

:

Simple Summary

Definitions and usage of the terms short-term, temporary, and transitional are pivotal to animal husbandry and welfare. English Government guidance regarding acceptable short-term, temporary, or transitional accommodation for animals varies widely from <1 day to 3 months; whereas independent scientific criteria and guidance typically use periods of hours to several days. Stipulations regarding acceptable short-term accommodations, notably among English Government guidance, are highly inconsistent and lack scientific rationale. The definitions and use of the terms short-term, temporary, and transitional (for both formal and other guidance) should be limited to precautionary time frames within one circadian cycle, i.e., periods of <24 h. At ≥24 h, all animals at all facilities should be accommodated in conditions that are consistent with long-term housing, husbandry, and best practices.

Abstract

The terms short-term, temporary, and transitional are related but can have different contexts and meanings for animal husbandry. The definitions and use of these terms can be pivotal to animal housing and welfare. We conducted three separate literature searches using Google Scholar for relevant reports regarding short-term, temporary, or transitional animal husbandry, and analysed key publications that stipulate relevant periods of accommodation. English Government guidance regarding acceptable short-term, temporary, or transitional accommodation for animals varies widely from <1 day to 3 months; whereas independent scientific criteria and guidance use typical periods of hours to several days. Stipulations regarding acceptable short-term, temporary, or transitional accommodation, notably among English Government guidance, which we focused on in this study, were highly inconsistent and lacked scientific rationale. The definitions and use of terms for both formal and other guidance should be limited to precautionary time frames within one circadian cycle, i.e., periods of <24 h. At ≥24 h, all animals at all facilities should be accommodated in conditions that are consistent with long-term housing, husbandry, and best practices.

1. Introduction

Animals are held captive for various reasons, e.g., as pets or companions, or for zoological, experimental, agricultural, rescue, and other purposes. Modern scientific research and practices unequivocally determine that animals require environments that meet a wide range of criteria, including ethological needs, behavioural choices, and environments that allow them to express a full repertoire of natural behaviours [1,2,3,4,5,6]. Whilst even the best captive environments in the most progressive zoos probably impose lifestyles of controlled deprivation on animals [7], the recognition and aims of keepers for long-term captive care should be to provide conditions that, at the very least, meet essential spatial, climatic, social, behavioural, nutritional, and other needs, as well as additional fundamentals integral to good welfare [8,9,10,11,12,13,14,15]. Inarguably, conditions that fail to meet such essential needs are associated with poor practices and result in reduced animal welfare [14,16,17].
Accordingly, certain conditions are compulsory for meeting essential welfare needs, and typically these conditions are set at standards of compliance with modern science and based on animals’ long-term (rather than temporary) husbandry requirements. Relatedly, in this context, both the husbandry conditions and the length of time during which an animal experiences them can be considered fundamentally relevant to its welfare state. Any situation affecting an animal must aim to maximise its welfare state and minimise negative effects. Therefore, in all situations where higher welfare standards (i.e., best practice conditions for long-term husbandry) are unlikely to be met, such as in short-term or temporary scenarios, protocols must rationally aim to minimise such periods.

Terminology

The terms ‘short-term’, ‘temporary’, and ‘transitional’ appear frequently in the literature and are applied arbitrarily. This situation leads to both confusion and laxity in their application. Whilst related, these terms can have different implications. For example, using standard Oxford Dictionaries for regular definitions, (1) short-term implies a brief period of time and indicates that change is soon anticipated; (2) temporary may imply brevity or indicate that a situation is impermanent and has an eventual yet open-ended future change; and (3) transitional indicates being in the process of change or transiting. Transportation conditions, whilst also characteristically short-term, temporary, and transitional, are typically considered separately and distinctly from best practice accommodation.
Accordingly, an animal at a veterinary clinic is housed short-term, whereas an animal at a zoo may be on temporary loan for several years, and an animal in a transitional situation may be in the process of being moved around, including transport to onsite or offsite situations. Relatedly, animals transiting via any facility, even when held for an unspecified period of time, could be considered transitional. Most animals at wholesale and retail centres would be considered transitional, regardless of the length of residence, because the intention is to receive or move them onwards. In this context, the term merely reflects temporary practices and cannot be regarded as a determination of what constitutes short-term or long-term animal husbandry. Therefore, the definition and use of ‘short-term’ is pivotal to establishing context and application for other descriptions that have temporal implications for husbandry.
The definition and consistent usage of the term ‘short-term’ would be helpful in coordinating and standardising all regulations involving animal care. Hereinafter, ‘short-term’ can be interpreted to include the terms temporary or transitional. This report aims to present some available information concerning current governmental and scientific guidance regarding the interpretation and application of short-term periods for the confinement of animals, as well as provide objective evidence-led recommendations for both uses of relevant terms and formal and informal policymaking.

2. Methods

A provisional search was conducted using the first 10 pages of Google and the terms short term + animal + housing + accommodation + government; temporary + animal + housing + accommodation + government; and transitional + animal + housing + accommodation + government. This provisional search was conducted in order to identify items that were primarily of government origin that may not appear in regular scientific literature searches. This provisional search identified 34 items. Following the exclusion of irrelevant items, 23 publications remained. Next, using the terms and strings presented in the Figure 1 Prisma diagram, we conducted additional separate literature searches in Google Scholar from general to more specific, in order to access the most appropriate parts of the literature. Each search string was entered into Google Scholar separately, and the results were combined. Any duplicates were removed. The abstracts and titles of the articles were read and scored for relevance to the topic. Articles that did not discuss short-term animal accommodation were removed. The numbers of articles shown refer to those remaining after de-duplication and screening for relevance was carried out.
The provisional Google search identified 14 government publications that contained husbandry guidance recommendations for animals, of which, 5 included information specific to short-term accommodation. From the Google Scholar searches, following the analysis of all initial relevant publications and exclusion of non-relevant items, 27 reports were identified for their inclusion of specific references to short-term, temporary, or transitional animal housing. References to short-term, temporary, or transitional animal housing were usually contained as recommendations within governmental guidance, and as incidental descriptions within independent peer-reviewed scientific articles.
We analysed all reports with regard to their status as government guidance or independent scientific projects, and recorded stipulations of short-term, temporary, or transitional determining references, according to animals of relevance, the context of the situation, and the periods interpreted (Table 1 and Table 2). In particular, we used materials published by the English and Welsh Governments to illustrate current formal guidance and associated issues concerning standards stipulated for short-term, temporary, or transitional accommodation of animals in a variety of situations. In this respect, most of the government regulations cited relate to the English Government’s Department of Environment, Food, and Rural Affairs (Defra).

3. Results

Table 1 provides summary information concerning animals, contexts, and stipulations regarding short-term confinement contained within governmental legislation or guidance, and Table 2 provides summary information contained within scientific peer-reviewed articles containing criteria or guidance regarding short-term accommodation and husbandry.
Figure 2 and Figure 3 provide a comparative context for information contained in Table 1 and Table 2.

4. Discussion

Government guidance information for short-term housing of animals (Table 1) was highly inconsistent, despite largely being produced by the same government department (Defra) in the same year. For example, the two formal guidance documents published by the English Government [18,21] essentially address the same groups of animals under comparable temporary husbandry conditions yet provide widely differing recommendations (short-term = 1 day maximum for holding mobile exhibition animals versus three months for holding animals at retail pet shops). It is worth emphasising that whilst the Welsh Government’s 2021 guidelines [19] adopted most of the English Government’s 2018 guidelines [18], the Welsh Government reduced the short-term stipulation from 3 months to 7 days. A further inconsistency in guidance is that under current English provisions, there are no stipulations at all for animals held at wholesale breeding or supply operations, and again there appears to be no scientific rationale or evidential support bases for this omission.
As a case study, snakes provide a pertinent example of inconsistency in English Government guidance. Under current English Government guidance for pet shops, snakes are the only vertebrate animals that can be kept or sold in conditions where they cannot fully stretch their bodies in accommodation for any defined time period [18]. In comparison, English Government guidance states that all animals (which includes snakes) kept for mobile exhibitions must be able to fully stretch their bodies even under temporary conditions (defined as 1 day) [21]. In contrast, in the Welsh Government’s 2021 [19] reinterpretation of the English Government’s 2018 [18] guidelines, snakes are expected to have the ability to fully stretch under any time-related accommodation. Notably, the English Government has acknowledged that it holds no supporting scientific evidence for this exception affecting snakes, and a formal government agency scientific review of the evidence concluded that snakes should be allowed to fully stretch their bodies [45], which is part of normal health maintenance and good welfare [46,47].
The exceptionally long (three months) definition of temporary or transitional issued by the English Government, solely in respect of animals held at pet shops, was queried with the responsible department (Defra), and formal rationale and supporting scientific evidence were requested. However, no formal response or scientific evidence was provided by the English Government to support its stipulation of three months to constitute temporary or transitional conditions. In comparison, to reiterate, the very similar Welsh Government guidance regulations were amended from stipulating 3 months to 7 days [19]. Manifestly, the English Government’s guidance for short-term accommodation is highly incongruous with normal scientific practices. The English Government has acknowledged prioritising input from vested interests, such as the pet and agricultural industries over independent scientific information, which could in part explain the emergence of arbitrary recommendations that aid some commercial practices rather than reflect scientific validity [48,49,50].
The stipulations for short-term contained within Table 1 represent specific guidance recommendations. However, certain periods and contexts for short-term animal housing contained within Table 2 are less defined and warrant further clarification. Accordingly, reference to ‘short-term captives to have all same benefits as long-term captives’ [24] is included because the original publication indicated that there should be no difference between short- and long-term animal accommodations (i.e., the better environment should be applied); and the reference to ‘long-term conditions applied to short-term housing’ [35] indicated that the study involved the default use of long-term accommodations as part of a short-term project.

4.1. The Acute Stress-to-Chronic Stress Transition Paradigm and its Relevance to Short-Term Housing

Stress, defined as a physiological response to potential or actual threats to the organism’s survival [51], is known to affect animals immediately on the impact of a stressor and may have acute or chronic effects [52,53,54]. Acute stress episodes (e.g., social competition, low-level predator-prey interactions, and minor injuries) are usually of short duration, i.e., <30 min [51], and typically fall within the normal coping limits of the individual [55,56,57,58,59], although some severe fearful stress events may produce life-long negative consequences [60]. There are many examples demonstrating how even single short-term stress episodes can have enduring negative consequences. For example, social stress among lizards has been found to impact animals for up to one week [61]. A review of stress (raised corticosterone) in reptiles found the following associations and duration of effects from acute stressors: exposure to salt water 1–4 weeks; social stress (hierarchy with dominating males) = 10–30 days; overcrowding = 10–14 days; and low relative humidity = 3 weeks [62].
Essentially, whilst some acute stress episodes may persist for several days or weeks, other stress-related disturbances to homeostasis caused by acute events can be holistically managed when followed by adequate periods of quiescence and normality. For example, handling an animal may cause it to experience acute stress, but if the animal is then allowed freedom from such interference and able to reside in appropriate shelter conditions, the individual may regain stability within hours or minutes [51,63]. However, at least two overarching considerations are important to this recovery scenario, namely the duration of stressors, and the quality of the recovery conditions.
Continuous stressors (such as invasive light or noise) [64], or multiple acute stress—‘microstress’—episodes [57,58], can result in serious negative health effects, because one episode may effectively rollover to another, resulting in cumulative stress impacts without adequate recovery. Such cumulative impacts from microstressors can facilitate the acute stress-to-chronic stress transition paradigm, resulting in dramatic disturbance of homeostasis and increased morbidity and mortality [54,55,56,57,58,59,65,66,67,68,69,70].
Essentially, all animals are biologically geared within the circadian cycle (a normal 24-h day) [71,72]. In nature, multiple microstressors are conceivable within each circadian cycle. The mechanisms (including physiological and temporal) regarding the transition from acute to chronic stress are not fully understood [72,73]. However, within each circadian cycle, animals naturally experience a normal rest and recovery period within familiar and evolutionarily relevant conditions (e.g., natural habitat, climate, social order) and, thus, such normality aids to facilitate and regain homeostasis [31,72,74]. Acute stressors and their effects that persist beyond a circadian cycle may exceed homeostatic coping limits and can thus be considered to constitute potential chronic stress events by rolling over into the next day, where an animal commences under a pre-existing stress burden [31,63]. Relatedly, disruption of circadian cycles is itself implicated in impaired biological functioning, increased morbidity, and decreased life spans [71,75,76].
An additional issue may be the species-specific speed of life, a factor that considers how a particular length of time may involve disproportionate effects on different species. For example, a circadian cycle may be proportionately (temporally) long for species (or individuals) that are small, short-lived, and have relatively fast metabolic rates compared with the same period applied to species that are large, long-lived, and have relatively slow metabolic rates [77]. Thus, a period of 24 hrs may pass more slowly for some animals, and this potential perceptual duration may be regarded as disproportionately important in a welfare context. Accordingly, and considering all issues thus far mentioned, a precautionary approach that may help to avoid the acute-to-chronic stress transition would be to minimise captivity-associated stressors from persisting beyond 24 hrs.
Moreover, unlike the natural environment, captive enclosures and associated husbandry methods probably involve numerous inherent environmental stressors (e.g., spatial restrictions, under-stimulation, poor social structures, and lack of habitat diversity, as well as abnormal dietary, thermal, lighting, humidity, sound, vibration, and other conditions) that are recognised stressors and could further elicit tipping points from acute-to-chronic stress situations. Accordingly, where single or multiple stressors impact animals in captive environments, the norms that promote homeostasis may be few or absent.

4.2. Managing Stressors and Stress

The nervous system of vertebrates detects, processes, integrates, and responds to a variety of external and internal stimuli [78,79]. Processing takes place in the central nervous system and is modulated by a variety of factors, such as age, hormones, and developmental history [78]. After processing, a variety of emergent responses occur that enhance biological fitness. Despite the diversity of species on earth, the foundations of neurobiology (‘afferent—processing—efferent’) are similar across species, as are the ultimate functions of maximising survival and reproduction ([78] p. 928). In addition to this broadly similar basic neurobiology, it must also be recognised that evolution will have selected for particular ‘subcortical neurocircuits’ ([78] p. 928) that produce species-specific physiological and behavioural responses. Learned behaviours will also impact the type, nature, and duration of responses, and indeed can be modified through classical and operant conditioning. Negative emotional states are evolutionarily adaptive, in that they protect the animal from situations that may threaten its survival and/or survival of its offspring [80]. On receipt of a painful or fear-eliciting stimulus animals will either freeze, fight or flee from the perceived danger. The response to stressors (such as fear) arises in two ways.
First, there is a short-term response often called ‘fight or flight’, where catecholamine hormones (dopamine, adrenaline, and noradrenaline) are released from the adrenal glands. These hormones prepare an organism for immediate action by increasing heart rate, widening air passages in the lungs, increasing blood pressure and glucose mobilisation, and numerous other physiological and behavioural effects, to allow the organism to overcome or flee from the dangerous situation [80,81]. The effects of adrenaline/noradrenaline are short-lived, lasting typically minutes to hours, after which the hormones are broken down, the organism’s arousal state returns to baseline, and homeostasis is restored [80]. In the wild, acute stressors can actually improve welfare by inducing appropriate adaptive responses, such as moving away from an aversive stimulus. However, a characteristic of long-term captive situations is the lack of behavioural choice; leading to chronic stress.
Second, there is a longer-term response mediated through glucocorticoid hormones, which take more time to exert their effects, and may have physiological consequences only 20–30 min after the stressful event [81]. If there is no further stressor, through a process of negative feedback, glucocorticoid levels start to drop within an hour; but the effects may last considerably longer [81]. This situation means that animals can recover from acute stressors more readily than chronic stressors. For example, in shelter dogs, cortisol levels were higher after 6 weeks in the shelter than on intake, where intake is likely to have been an acutely stressful event; the long-term kennel environment was clearly a chronic stressor [82]. There is also evidence (e.g., among fishes) that chronic long-term stress reduces the animals’ abilities to cope with subsequent short-term stressors [83]. Therefore, it is important to reduce chronic stress on animals so that they are more resilient to the acute stressors involved in the captive situation, for example, transport, handling, or medical treatments.
Animals occurring within particular sectors, for example, the commercial pet industry, are frequently subject to multifactorial acute and chronic stressors, including wild capture, intensive captive breeding, repeated handling, repeated overly-restrictive confinement and deprivation, local transportation, local transient storage, regional or international transportation, further transient local storage, and other situations (e.g., [84,85,86,87,88]). Accordingly, animals arriving at holding sites may already harbour significant cumulative stress burdens; thus, all efforts should be made to minimise additional negative pressures and affects that would likely derive from subnormal husbandry conditions. Studies show that animals transferred from outdoor to indoor facilities lose condition, likely partly due to increased stressors such as reduced cage sizes [85]. Clearly, animals ought to be provided as soon as possible with environments and protocols that offer the greatest opportunities for rest, recovery, and homeostatic stabilisation, which are implied in relation to environments of long-term best-practice husbandry. Failure to provide such opportunities is arguably tantamount to imposed deprivation and potential harm.
Whilst acute stress should not be an accepted consequence of short-term conditions, short-term housing situations should not cause chronic stress. Any situation that causes chronic (rather than acute) stress should not fall within the definition of short-term or temporary conditions. Short-term conditions should be interpreted as an absolute unavoidable minimum period during which, for overriding practical reasons, environments may not be fully consistent with the Five Freedoms [89], Five Domains [16,17], and other modern principles of welfare. For example, animals presenting at veterinary clinics or rescue facilities, arriving at formal animal import centres, or wholesale or retail pet businesses, may (for essentially brief periods) rationally be held in subnormal environments for specific purposes. Such purposes may include confinement in transportation enclosures, movement between enclosures, or being held in facilities as part of initial processing. Clearly, all such protocols should be pre-planned wherever possible and carried out rapidly in order to ensure that all animals are quickly transferred to higher standard conditions that are fully consistent with long-term husbandry.

5. Conclusions

Current terms and practices, relevant to the short-term (or temporary, transitional) housing of animals, whether for pets, display, or agriculture, involve highly inconsistent and largely arbitrary criteria. Such inconsistencies occur even when produced by a single responsible government entity, such as the United Kingdom’s Department of Environment, Food, and Rural Affairs (e.g., stipulations ranging from 12 h in catteries to 3 months in pet shops). Most examples of independent scientific criteria regarding short-term conditions included herein were derived primarily from rationalised research protocols resulting in incidental time scales for confinement, rather than particular recommendations. However, these independent scientific criteria manifestly and consistently adopted typical time periods of hours to several days as scientifically rational stipulations for short-term or temporary conditions of confinement.
Future guidance regarding the definition and use of short-term or temporary conditions requires two essential elements: first, a robust objective scientific rationale; and second, consistent application. Both these elements were lacking in the reviewed English Government and Welsh Government guidance; although in particular regarding information produced by the English Government. We recommend that the definition and use of the term short-term for both formal and other guidance should be limited to periods expressing time frames within the circadian cycle, thus limiting all stipulations in respect of short-term housing to infer periods of 24 h or less. A definition of <24 h for short-term accommodation is practically rational, already in use across governmental guidance, research protocols, and other applied situations, and is entirely generalisable. Moreover, importantly, periods of weeks, or months as stipulated by the English Government for short-term accommodation of animals in pet shops, are at risk of being unenforceable because it may not be possible for regulators to ascertain or validate the actual length of time animals are held within a facility (e.g., due to misplaced paperwork, record-keeping inadequacies, or the absence of individual identification measures). Three months of accommodation in inferior conditions is also clearly inconsistent with good welfare because animals may be subject to greatly prolonged lower standards of care throughout their entire occupation.

6. Recommendations

  • The stipulations for short-term, temporary, transitional, or other similarly intended conditions should infer periods of less than a single circadian cycle (typically <24 h).
  • All animals at all facilities should be subject to the single circadian cycle as a principle for determining maximum short-term, temporary, or other transitional conditions.
  • All animals at all facilities must be accommodated in higher or other similarly recognised conditions consistent with long-term husbandry and best practices wherever confinement persists beyond the single circadian principle.
  • Best practice examples of short-term, temporary, or other transitional conditions should include higher standards of husbandry.
  • Keeping animals in short-term, lower standard conditions should be minimised and only for recorded and essential reasons.
  • All animals at all facilities should be subject to government mandatory identification and registration on arrival and departure in order to accurately record their period of stay.

Author Contributions

Concept and design: C.W. and R.G.; literature research: R.G., C.W., C.S. and M.J.; analysis and writing: C.W., R.G., M.J. and C.S. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Not applicable.

Informed Consent Statement

Not applicable.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. French, F.; Williams, L.; Coe, J.C.; Single, L.; Fernandez, E.; Martin, C.F. ReptileJam 2021: Designing Enrichment for Reptiles in Captivity: This is a proposal for a ZooJam workshop around the theme of designing tech-enabled enrichment opportunities for zoo-housed and domestic reptiles. In Proceedings of the Eight International Conference on Animal-Computer Interaction (ACI’21), New York, NY, USA, 8–11 November 2021. [Google Scholar]
  2. Gygax, L.; Hillmann, E. “Naturalness” and its relation to animal welfare from an ethological perspective. Agriculture 2018, 8, 136. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Marliani, G.; Sprocatti, I.; Schiavoni, G.; Bellodi, A.; Accorsi, P.A. Evaluation of Horses’ Daytime Activity Budget in a Model of Ethological Stable: A Case Study in Italy. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2021, 24, 200–213. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Ryan, M.; Waters, R.; Wolfensohn, S. Assessment of the Welfare of Experimental Cattle and Pigs Using the Animal Welfare Assessment Grid. Animals 2021, 11, 999. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Sánchez-Casanova, R.; Sarmiento-Franco, L.; Phillips, C.; Zulkifli, I. Do free-range systems have potential to improve broiler welfare in the tropics? World’s Poult. Sci. J. 2020, 76, 34–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Zhuchaev, K.; Orlov, D.; Borisenko, E.; Kochneva, M.; Hart, V. Ethological evaluation of pig welfare under different keeping technologies in Siberian conditions. BIO Web Conf. 2021, 36, 06038. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Burghardt, G.M. Environmental enrichment and cognitive complexity in reptiles and amphibians: Concepts, review, and implications for captive populations. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2013, 147, 286–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. King, L.A. Behavioral evaluation of the psychological welfare and environmental requirements of agricultural research animals: Theory, measurement, ethics, and practical implications. ILAR J. 2003, 44, 211–221. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Baumans, V. Science-based assessment of animal welfare: Laboratory animals. Rev. Sci. Tech. 2005, 24, 503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Duncan, I.J. Science-based assessment of animal welfare: Farm animals. Rev. Sci. Tech. 2005, 24, 483–492. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Whitham, J.C.; Wielebnowski, N. Animal-based welfare monitoring: Using keeper ratings as an assessment tool. Zoo Biol. Publ. Affil. Am. Zoo Aquar. Assoc. 2009, 28, 545–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Alligood, C.A.; Dorey, N.R.; Mehrkam, L.R.; Leighty, K.A. Applying behavior-analytic methodology to the science and practice of environmental enrichment in zoos and aquariums. Zoo Biol. 2017, 36, 175–185. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  13. Alligood, C.; Leighty, K. Putting the “E” in SPIDER: Evolving trends in the evaluation of environmental enrichment efficacy in zoological settings. Anim. Behav. Cogn. 2015, 2, 200–217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Mendl, M.; Mason, G.; Paul, E.S. Animal welfare science. In APA Handbook of Comparative Psychology; Call, J., Burghardt, G.M., Pepperberg, I.M., Snowdon, C.T., Zentall, T., Eds.; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2017; Volume 2, pp. 793–811. [Google Scholar]
  15. Mendyk, R.W. Challenging folklore reptile husbandry in zoological parks. In Zoo Animals: Husbandry, Welfare and Public Interactions; Berger, M., Corbett, S., Eds.; CABI: Wallingford Oxon, UK, 2018; pp. 265–292. [Google Scholar]
  16. Mellor, D.J.; Beausoleil, N. Extending the ‘Five Domains’ model for animal welfare assessment to incorporate positive welfare states. Anim. Welf. 2015, 24, 241. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Mellor, D.J. Updating animal welfare thinking: Moving beyond the “Five Freedoms” towards “a Life Worth Living”. Animals 2016, 6, 21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  18. DEFRA. Selling Animals as Pets Licensing: Statutory Guidance for Local Authorities (Updated 2022). 2018. Available online: https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/animal-activities-licensing-guidance-for-local-authorities/selling-animals-as-pets-licensing-statutory-guidance-for-local-authorities--2 (accessed on 15 October 2022).
  19. Welsh Government. The Animal Welfare (Licensing of Activities Involving Animals) (Wales) Regulation 2021. Guidance Notes for Conditions for Selling Animals as Pets. 2021. Available online: https://gov.wales/sites/default/files/publications/2021-10/the-animal-welfare-licensing-of-activities-involving-animals-wales-regulation-2021-guidance.pdf (accessed on 15 October 2022).
  20. UK Government. Wildlife and Countryside Act. 1981. Available online: https://www.legislation.gov.uk/ukpga/1981/69 (accessed on 11 December 2020).
  21. DEFRA. Keeping or Training Animals for Exhibition Licensing: Statutory Guidance for Local Authorities. 2022. Available online: https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/animal-activities-licensing-guidance-for-local-authorities/keeping-or-training-animals-for-exhibition-licensing-statutory-guidance-for-local-authorities (accessed on 15 October 2022).
  22. UK Government. Guidance on the Operation of the Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986. 2014. Available online: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/662364/Guidance_on_the_Operation_of_ASPA.pdf (accessed on 15 October 2022).
  23. DEFRA. Cat Boarding Licensing: Statutory Guidance for Local Authorities. 2022. Available online: https://www.gov.uk/government/publications/animal-activities-licensing-guidance-for-local-authorities/cat-boarding-licensing-statutory-guidance-for-local-authorities (accessed on 15 October 2022).
  24. Broom, D.M. Indicators of poor welfare. Br. Vet. J. 1986, 142, 524–526. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Crane, M. Without the wisdom of Solomon or his ring: Setting standards for exhibited animals in New South Wales. J. Vet. Behav. 2007, 2, 223–229. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Hashizume, C.; Buote, P. Emergent Planning for the Veterinary Care and Short-Term Housing of Companion Animals Evacuated due to a Wildfire in Alberta, Canada. Prehospital Disaster Med. 2017, 32, S242. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Hobby, A.; Pankhurst, N.; Haddy, J. The effect of short term confinement stress on binding characteristics of sex steroid binding protein (SBP) in female black bream (Acanthopagrus butcheri) and rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss). Comp. Biochem. Physiol. Part A Mol. Integr. Physiol. 2000, 125, 85–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. McGarigal, C.R.; Galima, M.M.; Topping, D.; Goldman, K.; Cartamil, D.; Kelley, K.M.; Lowe, C.G. Physiological and behavioral effects of angling on California sheephead (semicossychus pulcher): Response, recovery, and captivity stress of an important gamefish in Southern California. J. Fish Res. 2020, 4, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Narayan, E.J.; Molinia, F.C.; Cockrem, J.F.; Hero, J.-M. Changes in urinary testosterone and corticosterone metabolites during short-term confinement with repeated handling in wild male cane toads (Rhinella marina). Aust. J. Zool. 2012, 59, 264–269. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Arena, P.C.; Warwick, C. Miscellaneous factors affecting health and welfare. In Health and Welfare of Captive Reptiles; Warwick, C., Frye, F.L., Murphy, J.B., Eds.; Chapman & Hall/Kluwer: London, UK; New York, NY, USA, 1995; pp. 263–283. [Google Scholar]
  31. Arena, P.C.; Bashaw, M.J.; Grant, R.; Howell, T.; Martínez-Silvestre, A.; Warwick, C. Miscellaneous factors for health and welfare. In Health and Welfare of Captive Reptiles, 2nd ed.; Warwick, C., Arena, P.C., Burghardt, G.M., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2023; pp. 583–617. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Tyrrell, C.; Cree, A. Relationships between corticosterone concentration and season, time of day and confinement in a wild reptile (tuatara, Sphenodon punctatus). Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 1998, 110, 97–108. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Ebrahimi, M.; Bull, C.M. Determining the success of varying short-term confinement time during simulated translocations of the endangered pygmy bluetongue lizard (Tiliqua adelaidensis). Amphibia-Reptilia 2013, 34, 31–39. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Mathies, T.; Felix, T.A.; Lance, V.A. Effects of trapping and subsequent short-term confinement stress on plasma corticosterone in the brown treesnake (Boiga irregularis) on Guam. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 2001, 124, 106–114. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  35. Adams, N.J.; Farnworth, M.J.; Rickett, J.; Parker, K.A.; Cockrem, J.F. Behavioural and corticosterone responses to capture and confinement of wild blackbirds (Turdus merula). Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2011, 134, 246–255. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Wichman, A.; Norring, M.; Pastell, M.; Algers, B.; Pösö, R.; Valros, A.; Saloniemi, H.; Hänninen, L. Effect of crate height during short-term confinement on the welfare and behaviour of turkeys. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2010, 126, 134–139. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Lombardo, M.P.; Thorpe, P.A. Captivity affects sperm production, testes size and beak color in house sparrows (Passer domesticus). Intern. Stud. Sparrows 2009, 33, 5–16. [Google Scholar]
  38. Mal, M.; Friend, T.; Lay, D.; Vogelsang, S.; Jenkins, O. Behavioral responses of mares to short-term confinement and social isolation. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 1991, 31, 13–24. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Weiler, U.; Salloum, B.A.; Claus, R. Influence of short-term confinement and exercise on tibia development in growing pigs. J. Vet. Med. Ser. A 2006, 53, 450–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Zhang, M.-Y.; Li, X.; Zhang, X.-H.; Liu, H.-G.; Li, J.-H.; Bao, J. Effects of confinement duration and parity on stereotypic behavioral and physiological responses of pregnant sows. Physiol. Behav. 2017, 179, 369–376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Zeiler, G.E.; Fosgate, G.T.; Van Vollenhoven, E.; Rioja, E. Assessment of behavioural changes in domestic cats during short-term hospitalisation. J. Feline Med. Surg. 2014, 16, 499–503. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  42. Griffin, B. Population wellness: Keeping cats physically and behaviorally healthy. In The Cat; Little, S.E., Ed.; W.B. Sauders: Philadelphia, PA, USA, 2012; pp. 1312–1356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Uetake, K.; Goto, A.; Koyama, R.; Kikuchi, R.; Tanaka, T. Effects of single caging and cage size on behavior and stress level of domestic neutered cats housed in an animal shelter. Anim. Sci. J. 2013, 84, 272–274. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. Judge, P.G.; de Waal, F.B. Conflict avoidance among rhesus monkeys: Coping with short-term crowding. Anim. Behav. 1993, 46, 221–232. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. AWC. Establishing Minimum Length Size for Snake Vivariums in Pet Selling Establishments (Working Title); Animal Welfare Commission: London, UK, (in press).
  46. Warwick, C.; Grant, R.; Steedman, C.; Howell, T.J.; Arena, P.C.; Lambiris, A.J.; Nash, A.-E.; Jessop, M.; Pilny, A.; Amarello, M. Getting It Straight: Accommodating rectilinear behavior in captive snakes—A review of recommendations and their evidence base. Animals 2021, 11, 1459. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Cargill, B.; Benato, L.; Rooney, N.J. A survey exploring the impact of housing and husbandry on pet snake welfare. Anim. Welf. 2022, 31, 193–208. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. McCullock, S.P. A Wolf in Sheep’s Clothing. 2020. Available online: https://theecologist.org/2020/feb/11/wolf-sheeps-clothing (accessed on 20 October 2022).
  49. Warwick, C.; Steedman, C. Exotic pet trading and keeping: Proposing a model government consultation and advisory protocol. J. Vet. Behav. 2021, 43, 66–76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. DEFRA (UK Department for Environment, Food & Rural Affairs); London, UK; Amess, D.; London, UK. Personal communication, 2019.
  51. Bayazit, V. Evaluation of cortisol and stress in captive animals. Aust. J. Basic Appl. Sci. 2009, 3, 1022–1031. [Google Scholar]
  52. Morgan, K.N.; Tromborg, C.T. Sources of stress in captivity. Appl. Anim. Behav. Sci. 2007, 102, 262–302. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Gelling, M.; Montes, I.; Moorhouse, T.P.; Macdonald, D.W. Captive housing during water vole (Arvicola terrestris) reintroduction: Does short-term social stress impact on animal welfare? PLoS ONE 2010, 5, e9791. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  54. Zhang, K. Environmental stressor, stress response, and disease. Environ. Dis. 2018, 3, 1. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Garner, J.P. Stereotypies and other abnormal repetitive behaviors: Potential impact on validity, reliability, and replicability of scientific outcomes. ILAR J. 2005, 46, 106–117. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  56. Moberg, G.P. Biological response to stress: Implications for animal welfare. In The Biology of Animal Stress: Basic Principles and Implications for Animal Welfare; Moberg, G.P., Mench, J.A., Eds.; CABI: Oxon, UK, 2000; pp. 1–21. [Google Scholar]
  57. Willner, P. The chronic mild stress (CMS) model of depression: History, evaluation and usage. Neurobiol. Stress 2017, 6, 78–93. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  58. Chmitorz, A.; Kurth, K.; Mey, L.K.; Wenzel, M.; Lieb, K.; Tüscher, O.; Kubiak, T.; Kalisch, R. Assessment of microstressors in adults: Questionnaire development and ecological validation of the Mainz inventory of microstressors. JMIR Ment. Health 2020, 7, e14566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Greenberg, N. The saurian psyche revisited: Lizards in research. In The Care and Use of Amphibians, Reptiles, and Fish in Research; Schaeffer, D.O., Kleinow, K.M., Krulisch, L., Eds.; Scientists Center Animal Welfare: Greenbelt, MD, USA, 1992; pp. 75–91. [Google Scholar]
  60. Zanette, L.Y.; Clinchy, M. Predator–prey interactions: Integrating fear effects. In APA Handbook of Comparative Psychology: Basic Concepts, Methods, Neural Substrate, and Behavior, Vol. 1; American Psychological Association: Washington, DC, USA, 2017; pp. 815–831. [Google Scholar]
  61. Korzan, W.J.; Summers, T.R.; Ronan, P.J.; Summers, C.H. Visible sympathetic activity as a social signal in Anolis carolinensis: Changes in aggression and plasma catecholamines. Horm. Behav. 2000, 38, 193–199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Martínez-Silvestre, A. How to assess stress in reptiles. J. Exot. Pet Med. 2014, 23, 240–243. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Gangloff, E.; Greenberg, N.B. Biology of stress. In Health and Welfare of Captive Reptiles, 2nd ed.; Warwick, C., Arena, P.C., Burghardt, G.M., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2023; pp. 93–142. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  64. Mancera, K.F.; Phillips, C.J.C. Effects of noise and light. In Health and Welfare of Captive Reptiles, 2nd ed.; Warwick, C., Arena, P.C., Burghardt, G.M., Eds.; Springer: Cham, Switzerland, 2023; pp. 357–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Breuning, L.G. Stimulating dopamine, serotonin, oxytocin and endorphin by learning how they’re stimulated in animals. J. Med. -Clin. Res. Rev. 2018, 2, 1–3. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Cowan, D.R. Adaptation, maladaptation and disease of captive reptiles. In Contributions to Herpetology; Murphy, J.B., Collins, J.T., Eds.; Society for the Study of Amphibians and Reptiles: Lawrence, KS, USA, 1980; pp. 191–196. [Google Scholar]
  67. Nelson, R.J.; Demas, G.E.; Klein, S.L.; Kriegsfeld, L.J. Seasonal Patterns of Stress, Immune Function, and Disease; Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  68. Schneiderman, N.; Ironson, G.; Siegel, S.D. Stress and health: Psychological, behavioral, and biological determinants. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol. 2005, 1, 607–628. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  69. Hoshaw, B.A.; Evans, J.C.; Mueller, B.; Valentino, R.J.; Lucki, I. Social competition in rats: Cell proliferation and behavior. Behav. Brain Res. 2006, 175, 343–351. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. Del Giudice, M.; Ellis, B.J.; Shirtcliff, E.A. Making sense of stress: An evolutionary—Developmental framework. In Adaptive and Maladaptive Aspects of Developmental Stress; Springer: Berlin/Heidelberg, Germany, 2013; pp. 23–43. [Google Scholar]
  71. Koch, C.; Leinweber, B.; Drengberg, B.; Blaum, C.; Oster, H. Interaction between circadian rhythms and stress. Neurobiol. Stress 2017, 6, 57–67. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  72. Oster, H. The interplay between stress, circadian clocks, and energy metabolism. J. Endocrinol. 2020, 247, R13–R25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  73. Douma, E.H.; de Kloet, E.R. Stress-induced plasticity and functioning of ventral tegmental dopamine neurons. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 2020, 108, 48–77. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  74. Russell, G.; Lightman, S. The human stress response. Nat. Rev. Endocrinol. 2019, 15, 525–534. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Martino, T.A.; Oudit, G.Y.; Herzenberg, A.M.; Tata, N.; Koletar, M.M.; Kabir, G.M.; Belsham, D.D.; Backx, P.H.; Ralph, M.R.; Sole, M.J. Circadian rhythm disorganization produces profound cardiovascular and renal disease in hamsters. Am. J. Physiol. Regul. Integr. Comp. Physiol. 2008, 294, R1675–R1683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  76. Evans, J.A.; Davidson, A.J. Health consequences of circadian disruption in humans and animal models. Prog. Mol. Biol. Transl. Sci. 2013, 119, 283–323. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Healy, K.; McNally, L.; Ruxton, G.D.; Cooper, N.; Jackson, A.L. Metabolic rate and body size are linked with perception of temporal information. Anim. Behav. 2013, 86, 685–696. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Coria-Avila, G.A.; Pfaus, J.G.; Orihuela, A.; Domínguez-Oliva, A.; José-Pérez, N.; Hernández, L.A.; Mota-Rojas, D. The Neurobiology of Behavior and Its Applicability for Animal Welfare: A Review. Animals 2022, 12, 928. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  79. Tort, L.; Cockrem, J.F.; Narayan, E.J. Editorial: Comparative endocrine stress responses in vertebrates. Comp. Endocr. Stress Responses Vertebr. 2020, 10, 652. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Ralph, C.; Tilbrook, A. Invited review: The usefulness of measuring glucocorticoids for assessing animal welfare. J. Anim. Sci. 2016, 94, 457–470. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  81. Romero, M.L.; Butler, L.K. Endocrinology of stress. Int. J. Comp. Psychol. 2007, 20, 89–95. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. van der Laan, J.E.; Vinke, C.M.; Arndt, S.S. Evaluation of hair cortisol as an indicator of long-term stress responses in dogs in an animal shelter and after subsequent adoption. Sci. Rep. 2022, 12, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Wunderink, Y.S.; Engels, S.; Halm, S.; Yúfera, M.; Martínez-Rodríguez, G.; Flik, G.; Klaren, P.H.; Mancera, J.M. Chronic and acute stress responses in Senegalese sole (Solea senegalensis): The involvement of cortisol, CRH and CRH-BP. Gen. Comp. Endocrinol. 2011, 171, 203–210. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Ashley, S.; Brown, S.; Ledford, J.; Martin, J.; Nash, A.E.; Terry, A.; Tristan, T.; Warwick, C. Morbidity and mortality of invertebrates, amphibians, reptiles, and mammals at a major exotic companion animal wholesaler. J. Appl. Anim. Welf. Sci. 2014, 17, 308–321. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  85. Fischer, C.P.; Romero, L.M. Chronic captivity stress in wild animals is highly species-specific. Conserv. Physiol. 2019, 7, coz093. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  86. Peng, S.; Broom, D.M. The Sustainability of Keeping Birds as Pets: Should Any Be Kept? Animals 2021, 11, 582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Sinclair, J.S.; Stringham, O.C.; Udell, B.; Mandrak, N.E.; Leung, B.; Romagosa, C.M.; Lockwood, J.L. The International Vertebrate Pet Trade Network and insights from US imports of exotic pets. BioScience 2021, 71, 977–990. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Grant, R.A.; Montrose, V.T.; Wills, A.P. ExNOTic: Should We Be Keeping Exotic Pets? Animals 2017, 7, 47. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Farm Animal Welfare Council. Farm Animal Welfare Council Press Statement. 1979. Available online: https://webarchive.nationalarchives.gov.uk/20121010012428/http://www.fawc.org.uk/pdf/fivefreedoms1979.pdf (accessed on 23 February 2020).
Figure 1. Search terms and strings for short-term, temporary, and transitional accommodation (each search was done separately, the results are combined, and the duplicates are removed). From: Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021, 372, n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71. For more information, visit http://www.prisma-statement.org/ (accessed on 30 January 2023).
Figure 1. Search terms and strings for short-term, temporary, and transitional accommodation (each search was done separately, the results are combined, and the duplicates are removed). From: Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, Boutron I, Hoffmann TC, Mulrow CD, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ 2021, 372, n71. doi: 10.1136/bmj.n71. For more information, visit http://www.prisma-statement.org/ (accessed on 30 January 2023).
Animals 13 00732 g001
Figure 2. Comparative context for information contained in Table 1. (References [18,19,20,21,22,23]).
Figure 2. Comparative context for information contained in Table 1. (References [18,19,20,21,22,23]).
Animals 13 00732 g002
Figure 3. Comparative context for information contained in Table 2. (References [24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44]).
Figure 3. Comparative context for information contained in Table 2. (References [24,25,26,27,28,29,30,31,32,33,34,35,36,37,38,39,40,41,42,43,44]).
Animals 13 00732 g003
Table 1. Short-term (including ‘temporary’ or ‘transitional’) confinement for animals: UK Government legislation or guidance.
Table 1. Short-term (including ‘temporary’ or ‘transitional’) confinement for animals: UK Government legislation or guidance.
Animal(s)ContextShort-Term StipulationSource
All animalsPet shops3 monthsEnglish Government [18]
All animalsPet shops7 daysWelsh Government [19]
All animalsWildlife72 hUK Government [20]
All animalsMobile exhibitions24 hEnglish Government [21]
All animalsLaboratory research situations<24 hUK Government [22]
CatsBoarding situations12 hEnglish Government [23]
Table 2. Short-term (including ‘temporary’ or ‘transitional’) confinement for animals: scientific criteria or guidance from peer-reviewed academic literature.
Table 2. Short-term (including ‘temporary’ or ‘transitional’) confinement for animals: scientific criteria or guidance from peer-reviewed academic literature.
Animal(s)ContextShort-Term StipulationSource
All animalsAll captive situationsShort-term captives are to have the same benefits as long-term captives[24]
All animalsExhibition situations<24 h[25]
Companion animalsClinical care situation11 h[26]
FishesPhysiological research situation<24 h[27]
Physiological research situation≤3 h[28]
AmphibiansPhysiological research situation24 h[29]
Reptiles generalAll captive situations24 h[30,31]
TuataraPhysiological research situation3 h[32]
LizardsTranslocation research situation1–5 days[33]
SnakesPhysiological research situation1 night[34]
BirdsCapture and confinement translocation or reintroduction research situation22 days (in aviaries) = long-term conditions[35]
Enclosure research situation6 h[36]
Physiological research situation<90 min[37]
HorsesSocial research situation48 h[38]
PigsHealth research situation2 weeks[39]
Physiological research situation1–15 days[40]
CatsHospitalisation behavioural research situation3–5 days[41]
Shelter husbandry research situation<2 weeks[42]
Shelter behavioural research situation6 days[43]
PrimatesZoo research situation1 day[44]
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Warwick, C.; Steedman, C.; Jessop, M.; Grant, R. Defining Short-Term Accommodation for Animals. Animals 2023, 13, 732. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13040732

AMA Style

Warwick C, Steedman C, Jessop M, Grant R. Defining Short-Term Accommodation for Animals. Animals. 2023; 13(4):732. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13040732

Chicago/Turabian Style

Warwick, Clifford, Catrina Steedman, Mike Jessop, and Rachel Grant. 2023. "Defining Short-Term Accommodation for Animals" Animals 13, no. 4: 732. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani13040732

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop